Image by Виталий Гуменюк |
For decades
science teachers have used the Peppered Moth (Biston betularia) as a prime example of natural selection. Prior to
the Industrial Revolution in Europe, the Peppered Moth population in England consisted
of a high percentage of light-colored salt and pepper moths (known as typica) and a low percentage of dark
moths (known as carbonaria). At that
time, the majority of trees were also light-colored due to the growth of
lichens on their bark, providing typicas
camouflage. Dark moths, it was presumed, were disproportionately spotted by
birds and eaten, thus keeping their population low.
As the
Industrial Revolution progressed, the lichens began to die and the tree trunks
became darker as they collected soot from the growing number of factories. At
the same time, the percentage of light moths began to decline, while carbonarias increased and became the
majority. The assumption of scientists was that now the darker moths had better
camouflage and the best adaptation to avoid predation, while the lighter moths
were now easy prey.
This is
classic Natural Selection: A given population (members of the same species
living in the same place and time) always contains a variety of phenotypes
(traits), some of which are better than others at helping an organism survive
long enough to reproduce and pass the adaptation to their offspring. However,
environmental changes (like pollution) can alter the balance, shifting greater fecundity
to organisms with a different phenotype, causing their proportion in the
population to increase.
The example
was so classic, so cut and dry, so obvious, that few scientists and even fewer
science teachers ever questioned its validity. The hypothesis was proposed as
early as 1896, according to The Scientist
(May, 2012) and validated in the 1950s by Bernard Kettlewell, who collected
compelling evidence that bird predation was in fact the selective force at work
and that moth camouflage was affected by pollution, by placing light and dark
moths directly on trees in polluted and unpolluted tracts.
However, in
the 1980s, Peppered Moth experts started to identify flaws in Kettlewell’s
experiments. Perhaps most compelling was their finding that tree trunks might
not be the moths’ preferred resting place, thus calling into question the whole
camouflage/bird predation hypothesis. It also threatened to make fools of the thousands
of science teachers who were still using the Peppered Moth as a prime example
of Natural Selection. Worse than this, however, was the field day it created
for creationists, who called the Peppered Moth story a fraud and an example of
scientists’ fallibility.
Enter
Michael Majerus, an evolutionary biologist from the University of Cambridge,
and a 50-year expert on Biston betularia.
Starting in 2001, according to The
Scientist, he set out to confirm Kettlewell’s findings using a more robust
and convincing protocol. First, through years of direct observation, he
discovered that the moths’ preferred resting site was the lateral branches of
trees (not their trunks). Then, rather than artificially placing moths in a
desired setting as Kettlewell did, he released thousands into an unpolluted
tract covered with nets (so they couldn’t escape and confuse migration with
predation).
Over the
course of seven years he found a 9% lower survival rate for carbonaria moths, indicating that they
indeed had a lower fecundity in an unpolluted setting and suggesting that they
were in fact being consumed at a higher rate.
Majerus died
in 2009 from an aggressive mesothelioma before his results could be published.
However, a detailed account of his work was published this year in Biology Letters (February 2012) by some
of his peers.
The lesson
for K-12 science teachers is that they need to stay up to date on scientific
discoveries and debates. In my experience, this does not happen often enough. Many
(if not the majority) of those who teach science have little, if any, practical
experience doing science in a real-world setting and few read scientific
journals with any regularity. Furthermore, it is very rare that they have time
to meet with colleagues to discuss new discoveries, review journal articles,
analyze methodologies, or debate controversies, something that is an important
cornerstone of university research.
No comments:
Post a Comment